ECOSYSTEMIC LINGUISTICS II
Hildo Honório do Couto
University
of Brasília, Brazil
Email: hildodocouto@gmail.com
Email: hildodocouto@gmail.com
0.
Introduction
Despite
the fact that Haugen is rightly considered the “father” of ecolinguistics, the
definition based on his ideas has several problems. First, it implies a
reification of language. Second, it sees language in relation to only the
social environment. Third, he uses ecological concepts as mere metaphors.
Fourth, he excludes what Makkai (1993) called endoecology of language, namely,
structural/grammatical phenomena from the domain of what would turn out to be
ecolinguistics. My purpose in this essay is to show that the way we are doing
ecolinguistics in Brasilia avoids all these problems.
The
members of the Brasilia School of Ecolinguistics see language as a web of
interactions, not a thing related to its environment and whose function is
communication. Further, there is not only an
“environment of language”, but at least four. They are the locus of the
interactions that make up language. We see language as embedded in ecosystems
(its exoecologies) and containing ecosystems in its interior. This means that
it is part of general ecology, and is ecology in itself. This means that
ecosystemic linguistics does not use ecological concepts as metaphors. Finally,
ecosystemic linguistics is a viewpoint from which it is possible to study both
exoecological and endoecological phenomena.
Section
1 of this essay will be dedicated to the discussion of why ecolinguistics is an
ecological discipline. At least in the way we do it in Brasilia (ecosystemic
linguistics) it is clearly a part of general ecology on a par with biological
ecology. In section 2 language will be seen as the verbal interactions that
take place inside the language ecosystem. In other words, this section will give
an overview of the ecology of communicative interaction, which is seen as the
core of language. Section 3 gives hints at how endoecological phenomena
(grammar, structure) can be approached from an ecolinguistic point of view. In
sections 4 and 5, some exoecological phenomena that can be dealt with will be
mentioned, such as those originally suggested by Haugen as well as those seen
in Alwin Fill’s work. Section 6 gives an overview of what has been done by
members of the Brasilia School of Ecolinguistics. It is pointed out that
ecosystemic ecolinguistics is a different way of doing linguistics, roughly in
line with Peter Finke’s ideas. Finally there are the Concluding Remarks in
section 7.
1.
Ecological and Linguistic Ecosystem: Ecosystemic Linguistics
Let
me begin by discussing the basic concepts of general ecology. It is well known
that its central concept is ecosystem. This consists of a population
(P) of organisms and their interactions (I) with the environment or territory
(T) as well as among themselves. The linguistic ecosystem has the
same structure. It consists of a population of individuals, or a people
(P), living in a certain place, which is their territory (T), and
interacting among themselves by means of the traditional way of interacting,
i.e., their language (L), equivalent to the interactions of the
biological ecosystem (I=L). The organism-environment (world) interactions of
the biological ecosystem are equivalent to signification, reference or
denotation in the linguistic ecosystem. The organism-organism interactions are
equivalent to communication. The interactions among phonemes forming a
syllable, of morphemes forming words, of words forming phrases and of phrases
forming sentences are equivalents of the relations that obtain in the internal ecology
of organisms, that is, among organs, cells and so on, as was suggested by
Trampe (1990: 84, 123).
Ecosystemic
linguistics does not pick up concepts from biological ecology and insert them
in language studies, a position first criticized by Garner (2004). Contrary to
traditional ecolinguistics, which uses them as mere metaphors, ecosystemic
linguistics sees language as a part of general ecology, as is the case
with other social sciences, such as environmental sociology, environmental
psychology, ecological anthropology and so on. Ecosystemic linguists
do a specific kind of ecology, namely, linguistic ecology, an
alternative name for ecolinguistics.
Alwin
Fill shows that there are two main strands of research in ecolinguistics. In
the first, “‘ecology’ is understood metaphorically and transferred to
‘language(s) in an environment’”. It was originally suggested by Haugen in
1970. Among the other researchers of this line he also mentions Peter Finke,
Wilhelm Trampe and, up to a certain point, Adam Makkai, as well as some others
such as Jørgen Chr. Bang and Jørgen Døør. According to the second view,
“‘ecology’ is understood in its biological sense; the role of language in the
development and aggravation of environmental (and other societal) problems is
investigated; linguistic research is advocated as a factor in their possible
solution” (Fill 2001: 43). The starting point of this view was a talk given by
Halliday at the AILA 1990 Conference, reproduced in Halliday (2001). This trend
is also followed by Richard Alexander, Andrea Gerbig, Andrew Goatly, Martin
Döring and many others. In an earlier version of Fill’s paper (Fill 1996b: 14),
he suggests a possible dialectical integration of the two approaches in a kind
of “integrational ecolinguistics” (integrative Ökolinguistik).
Ecosystemic linguistics is also “integrational”, although it does not transfer
concepts from one side to the other. On the contrary, it sees ecolinguistics as
a branch of general ecology, to the point that, as we have just seen, an alternative name for it is linguistic
ecology (ecologia linguistica, in Portuguese), in which ‘ecology’ is
the head of the noun phrase, whereas ‘linguistic’ is the complement. A good German term for it would be Sprachökologie.
Albert
Bastardas i Boada and Norman Denison also deal with the distribution of
languages in space and their interactions from an ecosystemic perspective.
According to Bastardas i Boada (2000: 22, my translation), “the existence of
linguistic varieties is approached from the ecosystemic point of view”.
According to him, “linguistic systems are probably in a state of unstable
equilibrium, given their condition of open systems – exchanging information and
energy with their environs” (p. 89). A question not asked by him is why Catalan
shares some features with Spanish and others with French, a fact that is due to
its being between the two. Denison (2001) investigated the languages of Europe
as constituting a vast ecosystem (ecology).
The
idea of language as an ecosystem embedded in a wider ecosystem is implied in
practically every writing by Peter Finke, at least in those I know of. He is
followed by his former student Wilhelm Trampe, who used the term “linguistic
ecosystems” (linguistische Ökosysteme) (Trampe 1990; 11). However, as
far as I could investigate, the first author to use the term “ecosystemic
linguistics” in print, albeit following ideas put forward by Finke, was Hans
Strohner. He presents ‘the computer metaphor', ‘the brain metaphor' and ‘the
ecosystem metaphor'. According to him, the third metaphor is more in accordance
with the needs of our time. The expression ökosystemische Sprachwissenschaft
appears in the title of his paper (Strohner 1996). In the Methology section of
the paper the alternative expression ökosystemische Linguistik appears
four times. In this connection it is of interest to mention Halliday’s
“systemic-functional grammar”. Ecosystemic linguistics recognizes three
language ecosystems, namely, the natural, the mental and the social. The three
are included in the all-encompassing integral ecosystem of language, formerly called "fundamental ecosystem of language". Let me
briefly discuss each of them.
On
the road from nature to culture, the first ecosystem to be mentioned is the natural
ecosystem of language. It is exactly parallel to the biological ecosystem,
as we saw above. It consists of a people (P1), whose members live in
their community’s territory (T1) and speak their own language (L1).
Inside this ecosystem, P1 and T1 form the natural
environment of language. This environment is the locus
where the physical, natural and/or biological interactions of language take
place. In this case, P1 and T1 are concrete
entities, so that P1 could be exemplified by the Tukano people of
the Upper Rio Negro in Brazil, T1 represents the territory where this
people is located, and L1 is its specific language, Tukano.
Everything having to do with language as a natural-biological phenomenon
belongs here. This is the first of four linguistic ecosystems. For this reason it was
indicated by the index 1.
The
second ecosystem is the mental ecosystem of language. If we understand mind
as the functioning of the brain, we may represent it with P2, a part
of ‘people/person’. These are fundamentally the neuronal connections where
language is formed, stored and processed. The locus of the mental
interactions (mind), i.e., the brain, may be represented by T2.
L2 represents language as a mental phenomenon. P2 and T2
together, especially former, make up the mental environment of language. The knowledge of
inter-word, inter-morphemic (morphosyntax) and inter-phonemic (phonology)
interactions as well as the knowledge of rules in general belongs to this
ecosystem. It is the only one recognized by generative grammar, as well as by
other mental theories of language.
The
third ecosystem is the social ecosystem of language. It consists of the
totality of the individuals of the community qua social beings,
inter-individualities or inter-subjectivities. In this ecosystem, L3
represents language as a social phenomenon. P3 represents all the
social individuals who constitute the collectivity. T3
represents the “organization” where this collectivity is located, in short, it
is the locus of the social interactions via language, that is to say, society.
Collectivity plus society constitute the social environment of language.
These
three ecosystems are embedded in the all-encompassing integral ecosystem
of language. It consists of people (P), territory (T) and language (L) at
large, not of a specific P, T or L, as in the case of the natural ecosystem of
language. This ecosystem is also known by at least two alternative
designations. The first is foundational ecosystem of language because it
also corresponds to the lay-person’s view of language. When he/she hears the
name of a language for the first time, the first question to come to his/her
mind is: “Which people (P) speaks it?” Upon hearing the answer, the second
question is: “Where (T) do this people live?”.
I
would like to add that these triads (P, T, L) are not only reminiscent of
Peirce’s sign components but also of Trampe’s (1990: 190) interpretation of
ideas by Albert Bandura and Kurt Lewin. Trampe’s P of Person ‘people’
corresponds to population or people. His V, of Verhalten ‘behavior’,
corresponds to L. Finally, the U of Umwelt ‘environment, territory’ in
his figure is homologous to territory.
All
the discussion in this section shows that most theories of language are partial.
Some of them focus only on the relation between language and world,
emphasizing reference. This tradition
sees only the relationships involving the natural environment of language.
Other theories consider language as an exclusively mental phenomenon, as does
generative grammar and the whole philosophical tradition it is based on. They
deal only with the interactions between language and its mental environment.
Finally, there are theories of language for which language is an exclusively
social phenomenon, as is the case with modern sociolinguistics and discourse
analysis. Although this point of view is frequently shared by common sense, it
is partial too. From the ecosystemic-ecolinguistic point of view, language is
all that. It is a biopsychosocial phenomenon.
Outside linguistics, these distinctions are relatively common. For example, Guattari
(1989) recognizes three “écologies” (= ecosystems), namely, the environmental
(natural), the social and the mental. These are included in an “écosophie”,
which is different from Arne Naess’ ecosophy. In any case it corresponds to the
integral ecosystem of language. Using the term “ecology” instead of
“ecosystem”, the Brazilian philosopher Boff (2012) shows himself a bit more "ecosystemic" because he talks about four ‘ecologias”, namely, the
“environmental/natural”, the “mental” and the “social” ecology. The three are enclosed
in the “integral ecology”, again equivalent to the integral ecosystem of
language. In the realm of ecolinguistics, Jøgen Døør and Jørgen Christian Bang
are probably the only ones to come close to our view. They recognize at least
the first three ecosystems in their “three dimensions”, that is, “bio-logics”
(natural), “ideo-logics” (mental) and “socio-logics” (social), as can be seen
all along Bang & Døør (2007), but first presented in Døør & Bang
(1996).
The
integral ecosystem of language, or community, may be seen from two different
perspectives, namely, speech community and language community. Speech
community (French: communauté de parole, German Sprechgemeinschaft, Interaktionsgemeischaft, Kommunikationsgemeinschaft) consists of a few people
(P), sharing a place -- a small territory (T) -- and interacting on a daily basis by
means of the usual internal way of interacting (L). Wherever there are people
communicating this way there is a speech community. As is the case with
biological ecosystems, speech community is delimited by the investigator. Its
size may range from only speaker and hearer in conversation, a minimal speech community,
to a whole continent or more, as is the case with the totality of regions of
the world where people communicate in
Portuguese (Portugal, Brazil, Angola, Mozambique, Cape Verde, São Tomé
and Príncipe, and East Timor), a maximal speech community. This maximal speech community is a language community (Sprachgemeinschaft). It comprizes all the regions of the world where the language in question is used, resembling the biomes of biological ecology, as is the case with taiga, tundra and rainforest, whose limits are not defined by the investigator. They are roughly there -- in nature --, independently of ecologists. A speech
community may be simple or complex. Complex speech communities
obtain where the linguistic ecosystem is bi-/multilingual and/or
multidialectal. Simple speech communities are monolingual and
monodialectal. However, these are the exception, not the rule.
As to language community, it is the regions of the world whose inhabitants know that the local way of communicating is language x, whether it is presently used or not. Up to a certain point, it is equal to the maximal speech community. In summary, language community is language seen from the perspective of the system, whereas in speech community it is seen from the perspective of communicative interaction.
Finally, I would like to give a concrete example that illustrates the usefullness of these concepts. For example, in Southern África, Sesotho (L) roughly means the way members of the Basotho people (P), who lives in the Lesotho country (T), communicate.
As to language community, it is the regions of the world whose inhabitants know that the local way of communicating is language x, whether it is presently used or not. Up to a certain point, it is equal to the maximal speech community. In summary, language community is language seen from the perspective of the system, whereas in speech community it is seen from the perspective of communicative interaction.
Finally, I would like to give a concrete example that illustrates the usefullness of these concepts. For example, in Southern África, Sesotho (L) roughly means the way members of the Basotho people (P), who lives in the Lesotho country (T), communicate.
2.
The Ecology of Communicative Interaction
As
Eugenio Coseriu has always emphasized, language is basically communicative
interaction (habla). More precisely, interaction is the core of
language. The system is an abstraction drawn from it by the linguist (Coseriu
1967). Even grammar exists to facilitate understanding during communicative
interactions. In certain circumstances there may be some understanding even in
the absence of a common grammar. This can be seen in attempts at communication,
such as in inter-lingual contacts. If the parties share at least a small list
of words, a precarious understanding can take place, as was certainly the case
with the first contacts that led do the formation of pidgins and creoles.
The
ecology of communicative interaction comprises a setting or scenario,
whose components vary from one case to the other. Among the fixed components we
have a speaker (I) and a hearer (YOU), who are parties in the communicative
interaction, or dialogue. If the speaker includes the hearer, we have
inclusive WE. He may also include the person(s) who is(are) on his side, that
is, S/HE1 as well those who are on the side of the hearer, namely,
S/HE2. The sum of the two gives THEY. If the speaker includes only
S/HE1, and excludes YOU, we have exclusive WE. If he does include YOU
we have inclusive WE. All these deictic forms, except I, may be replaced by all
the nouns of the language. In this vein, pronouns do not replace nouns, but the
other way round, it is the nouns that replace pronouns because language was born and exists in acts of communicative interaction.
Spatial,
temporal and modal deixis also belong here. For instance, here, now and this
way are associated with I. In other words, the whole lexicon of the
language emerges out of the ecology of communicative interaction. There are
also the verbs designating actions, and the adjectives qualifying the entities,
besides linking words such as prepositions and conjunctions.
Another
type of component of communicative interaction is some rules that make
understanding possible. Ecosystemic linguistics recognizes at least two types
of rules, namely systemic and interactional rules. Systemic rules are
basically the rules of grammar. They are studied by structural theories, like generative
grammar. Interactional rules are less studied in linguistics. However,
conversation analysis, Grice’s maxims, Austin’s speech acts with their
performative values, as well as sociological theories of social action and philosophical
action theory touch on them occasionally. Grice’s
cooperative principle has to do with what in ecosystemic linguistics is called communion,
that is, a mindset that keeps two or more people together, and happy with this
simple being together. It is a prerequisite for successful communication.
Communion is a special type of communication, which Malinowski called “phatic
communion”. The speaker expresses him/herself the way s/he thinks will be
understood by the listener, whereas the listener interprets the speaker’s words
the way s/he thinks was intended by the speaker. Without communion there is no
communicative interaction. It is not enough to share
rules. A willingness to communicate is also necessary.
Let
us come back to interactional rules. Some of them are highly culture-dependent.
Some others have a universal or near-universal character. Each culture has its
own set of rules. Up to now, we have come up with about 13 interactional
rules for Brazilian Portuguese. They must be strictly obeyed if communication
is to flow smoothly. They are the following:
1)
Speaker and hearer must be near one another, roughly one meter (this is culture-bound).
2)
The exchange must be face-to-face.
3)
Speaker and hearer must look at the face of one another, if possible at the
eyes.
4)
Every appeal must be complied with.
5)
Both appeal and compliance must be formulated in a cooperative, harmonious,
solidary tone.
6)
Every appeal must be preceded by a kind of pre-appeal, a vocative, such as please!,
hi!
7)
Turn-taking is obligatory, although sometimes speaker and hearer speak
simultaneously, which is not welcome (culture-bound).
8)
If the subject is serious, speaker and hearer must show this in their facial
expression; if it is light, a sign of lightness, as a smile, is welcome.
9)
Speaker and hearer must keep themselves attentive during the interaction,
without distractions, “side lookings” and so on.
10)
Especially on telephone speaker and hearer must signal that they are there
(hm-hm, OK, yes)
11)
The interaction may not be interrupted abruptly. The participant who intends to
end it must signal his/her intention.
l2)
As a rule, it is he/she who began the interaction that should end it. The contrary would look
non-cooperative, non-harmonious.
13) Systemic rules (grammar, structure).
13) Systemic rules (grammar, structure).
As we can see, grammar (systemic rules) is a part of interactional rules because they, too, exist in order to enhance understanding. Conventional linguistics sees these two types of rules the other way round. According to them, conversation is possible only as putting into action the "rules" of grammar. Whenever a member of the community wants to address another, he picks some of these "rules", forms "phrases" and send them to the hearer, inverting what Coseriu had shown several decades ago. The
communicative interaction itself is the exchange of an appeal by the
speaker and a compliance by the hearer. The most “primitive” appeal is command.
Cases like get out! and leave me alone! may also be rendered only by
gestures and/or physical action (jostling, shoving, push). A second type of
appeal is question, which requires an answer. A third type is vocative,
a kind of calling the potential hearer to get in communion with the speaker, so
that communicative interaction becomes possible. A fourth type is exclamation.
The fifth is information (declarative sentence), which is a response to
an appeal or request, even if tacit. We only give a piece of information to
somebody else when s/he asks for it or when we think s/he needs it. If s/he
were aware of the fact that s/he needed this information, s/he would ask for
it. In general all information or
declarative sentence is an answer to a (tacit) question (Maas 1977). These
exchanges occur in the dialogue, or flow of communicative interaction.
In
summary, seeing language from the ecology of communicative interaction avoids
some pitfalls. First of all, we can see that language does not interact with
environment directly. It is the members of the people who interact among
themselves in it. Language is a specific type of interaction that takes place
inside the linguistic ecosystem. As Fill (1993: 5) put it, language is not the
river but the current, the flow, itself (das Fliessen selbst). The second
most important consequence of this view is that language is not reified. It is
not a “thing” interacting with its environment. The third is that language is
not an instrument (a thing) of communication. It is communication.
3.
The endoecology of Language
We
know that in biological ecology organisms are not only part of an ecosystem;
they have ecosystems inside themselves. Most ecolinguists have dealt
exclusively with exoecological interactions. Linguistic ecology, an alternative
name for ecosystemic linguistics, tries to study language from all sides, as far
as possible, albeit using different methodologies wherever needed, as indicated by Garner (2004). Like all
ecological disciplines, ecosystemic linguistics is multidisciplinary. It
includes the endoecology of language in its domain, namely, structural
subjects, including lexicon, grammar and semantics.
Finke (1996: 38) said that “language
[…] has a structural organization ecosystemically describable”. According to
him, language rules emerges out of so-called laws of nature (p. 40). He
explicitly mentions phonetics, phonology, syntax and semantics (p. 41).
Elsewhere he says that “in ecolinguistics, the mere analysis of talk on
environmental issues – which is sometimes taken to be the subject of
ecolinguistics – in my opinion falls short of the possibilities and necessities
for a creative innovation of ecolinguistics” (Finke 2001: 88). In Mühlhäusler (2003) there are
interesting reflections on grammar from an ecolinguistic point of view. Trampe
(1990: 84) says, referring to Bateson: “let us compare grammar with the anatomy
of an organism, a plant for example". Some pages below he adds that “languages
are simply part of language-world-systems. Grammar is a system based on
biological design, according to which linguistic elements must combine” (p.
123).
Several
endoecological subjects have been dealt with by members of the Brasília School
of Ecolinguistics as can be seen in section 6. One of them is semantics,
illustrated with spatial prepositions. Another is syntax,
especially the order of constituents and/or agreement among them, as in the case
of person and/or gender agreement in Portuguese. In morphology, some word
formation processes have been investigated, as the prefixes re-/des-
(re-de/un) as well as the shortening of circumlocutions to a disyllabic word.
However, one of the most investigated domains is the lexicon.
Ecosystemic
linguistics includes grammar in its object of study, but as an aid communicative interaction. Let us take a look at the famous
phrase Colorless green ideas sleep furiously, proposed by Chomsky in
1957. The reason for its strangeness is exactly the opposite of what he
believed: it is because it does not correspond to anything in the world. As the
“first” Wittgenstein would say, it does not describe anything outside
language.
4.
The exoecology of Language
Ecolinguistics
was born as a reaction against structuralist exclusive endoecological approach.
This is clear in Alwin Fill’s early works. The subjects Haugen suggested are
all exoecological. In a perfunctory survey of extant readings in
ecolinguistics, I could see that about 62%
of them are some kind of discourse analysis. Circa 94% of the texts
published in the excellent site www.ecoling.net moderated by Arran
Stibbe also have to do with discourse analysis.
Ecosystemic
linguistics also privileges exoecological questions. Departing from the
ecosystem and its components and features, the most diverse subjects have been
investigated. One of them is the ecology of languages, as was done by
Denison (2001), Bastardas i Boada (2000), Calvet (1999) and many others. Among
the subjects that have been dealt with under this heading, we could mention
bilingualism and multilingualism, language contact (including pidginization and
creolization), language planning, language policy, Sprachbund phenomena
and so on (Haugen 1972). Couto (2009) is dedicated to the ecology of
language contact.
Another
topic that has been approached from an exoecological optic is the ecology of
language evolution. Mufwene (2001) is one of its most conspicuous
representatives, although he does not consider himself an ecolinguist. However,
the title of his book speaks for itself. His point of departure is population
genetics. Homologizing language with species, not organism, he sees the causes
of evolution in the Darwinian ideas of competition and selection, via imperfect
replication of the language by each generation. Finke (2008) also investigates
questions such as the memory of language, together with cultural evolution.
Language
ethnoecology also belongs to the realm not only of ecosystemic
linguistics. The group of investigators gathered around the NGO Terralingua is
a case in point, as can be seen in the essays published in Maffi (2001). Couto (2007: 219-280) has suggested that language ethnoecology be incorporated in ecosystemic linguistics.
Language
acquisition is an important side of language phenomena. Ecolinguists have
hardly dealt with it but the ecology of language acquisition must be
incorporated in linguistic ecology. Outside ecolinguistics we have a few investigations
as those discussed in the several papers contained in Kramsch (2002) and
Leather & van Dam (2003). Closely associated with language acquisition is
the general discipline of applied linguistics, in whose context ecolinguistics
was born in Europe. The subtitle of one of the latest collective books (Fill
& Penz 2007) is Essays in Applied Ecolinguistics. Fill himself is a
specialist in the area. Finally, the famous and thought-provoking conference
paper of Halliday (2001) was delivered at an AILA meeting (see Halliday
2001).
Topics
of environmental linguistics have had a privileged place in
ecolinguistics, albeit under names such as critical ecolinguistics and ecocritical
linguistics. Ecosystemic linguists are developing a specific type of
discourse analysis, namely ecological discourse analysis. This will be
discussed in the next section.
5
Ecological Discourse Analysis
Most
trends in discourse analysis are firmly rooted in ideology, especially Marxist
ideology, and power relations. Ecological discourse analysis (EDA) takes
a radically different position. Instead of ideology and power relations, it
bases itself on the preservation of life on earth and on an avoidance
of suffering. Its theoretical sources are Arne Naess’ deep ecology and
Gandhi’s ideas. Since ideologies are inevitable, EDA departs from an ideology of life, ecological ideology, or ecoideology (Couto 2013). From here on it is possible to end up in political ideologies.
Let
us see the case of a woman suffering in the hands of a drunken husband who
beats her constantly. EDA defends her not for being a woman, not exactly in the name of feminism, but because she
is a human (and animal) being who is suffering. Defending her from the point of
view of feminism would be a kind of discrimination from the upside down or the
wrong way round. The EDA sees in the man her partner, not her antagonist.
The
same principle applies to cases of infanticide among some ethnic groups of
Brazil. Ecological discourse analysts are against this practice because it
implies the maximum suffering, death. Some anthropologists say that we cannot
interfere because it is generally an ancient cultural practice. For EDA, however, cultural
practices can change along history, whereas death is irreversible. Besides
that, these groups are already totally or partially acculturated. Whenever
possible, we ought to save children from this practice. If possible without causing suffering in the community.
There is not only physical suffering, but also mental (torture)
and social suffering, as when a person is ridiculed publicly. In this case, we
must ponder which suffering is the hardest (for more on EDA, see the in this blog especially dedicated to it).
6.
The Brasília School of Ecolinguistics
The
starting point for the Brasilia School of Ecolinguistics was Couto (1999), in
which the subject ‘ecolinguistics’ was discussed for the first time in Brazil.
Then two courses were taught at the graduate level and one at the undergraduate
level, respectively. Two M.A. theses and, subsequently, two Ph.D. theses were written. At the
present moment, there are several Ph.D. students working on ecolinguistic
subjects. One of them is working on the adaptation of rural dialect speakers to
the speech of urban Brasília2. A second one is investigating
Portuguese in the language ecology of East Timor3. A third one is
conducting research in an ex-maroon community of northern Brazil4.
He investigates how members of this conservative community designate plant
species of their environment, and whether this ethnobotanical knowledge is
being transmitted to new generations.
The
next undertaking was the publication of Couto (2007), a thick book giving an
overview of ecosystemic-linguistic subjects. One of its main innovations, if
any, is the suggestion to include in ecolinguistics what has been done in
ethnosciences, although this had already been done by the ecolinguist
Mühlhäusler (2001) as well as by most contributors to Maffi (2001). In 2009, I
published a second ecolinguistics book (Couto 2009), which tries to show how
language contact can be handled in the framework of our discipline, with an
emphasis on the movement of populations in space. In the same year, Elza K. N.
N. do Couto, professor at the University of Goiânia, initiated a postdoc
program in Brasília under my supervision. She investigated the language and
culture of a small community of Kalderash Gypsies. She introduced
ecolinguistics as a regular discipline of the graduate program of linguistics.
She is also supervising several theses on ecolinguistics subjects.
7.
Concluding Remarks.
In
summary, for ecosystemic linguistics, ecolinguistics is a new way of doing
linguistics. This means that the investigator can deal with any language
phenomena, even using the same methodology s/he used before, because
ecolinguistics is multi-methodological. The difference lies in the fact that
s/he conducts her/his research from a unified point of view, the ecological
point of view, as suggested by Finke (2008).
References
Bang,
Jørgen Chr. & Døør, Jørgen. 1996. Language, ecology & truth – Dialogue
& dialectics. In: Fill (ed.)
1996a, p. 17-25.
_______.
2007. Language, ecology and society: A dialectical approach. London: Continuum.
Bastardas i Boada, Albert. 2000. Ecologia
de les llengües. Barcelona: Proa, 2nd ed.
Boff, Leonardo. 2012. As
quatro ecologias: ambiental, política e social, mental e integral. Rio de
Janeiro: Editora Mar de Ideias.
Calvet,
Louis-Jean. 1999. Pour une écologie des langues du monde.
Paris: Plon.
Coseriu, Eugeniu. 1967. Teoría
del lenguaje y linguistica general. Madrid: Gredos, 2nd ed.
Couto, Elza K. N. do. 2013. Ecolinguística:
Um diálogo com Hildo Honório do Couto. Campinas: Pontes.
Couto, Hildo Honório do. 1981.
Codificação linguística de eventos. In: Couto et al. (eds.). Ensaios de linguistic aplicada ao português.
Brasília: Thesaurus, p. 51-78.
_______.
1999. Contato interlinguístico: Da interação à gramática. Available at:
www.ecoling.unb.br (access: 20/l2/2016).
www.ecoling.unb.br (access: 20/l2/2016).
_______. 2007. Ecolinguística: Estudo
das relações entre língua e meio ambiente. Brasília: Thesaurus.
_______. 2008. Algumas restrições aos
proparoxítinos em português. In: Roncarati, C. & Abrassado,
J. (eds.).
2008. Português brasileiro II: contato lingüístico, heterogeneidade e história. Niterói: EDUFF/FAPERJ,
2008, p. 118-136.
_______. 2009. Ecologia,
linguística e ecolinguística: Contato de línguas: São Paulo: Contexto.
_______. 2012. O tao da
linguagem: Um caminho suave para a redação. Campinas: Pontes.
_______. 2013b. Análise do
discurso ecológica. In: https://ecosystemic-linguistics.blogspot.com.br
Denison,
Norman. 2001. A linguistic ecology for Europe. In: Fill & Mühlhäusler
(eds.): 75-83.
Döring,
Martin, Hermine Penz & Wilhelm Trampe (eds. 2008). Language, Signs and
Nature. Ecolinguisic
Dimensions of Environmental Discourse. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.
Fill,
Alwin. 1987. Wörter zu Pflugscharen – Versuch einer Ökologie der Sprache.
Vienna: Böhlau.
_______.
1993. Ökolinguistik: Eine Einführung. Tübingen: Gunter Narr.
Fill,
Alwin (ed.).1996a. Sprachökologie und
Ökolinguistik. Tübingen: Stauffenburg Verlag, p. 49-58.
_______. 1996b. Ökologie der
Linguistik – Linguistik der Ökologie. In: Fill (ed.), p. 3-16.
_______
& Peter Mühlhäusler (eds.) 2001. The Ecolinguistics Reader. London:
Continuum.
Fill,
Alwin (2001). Ecolinguistics. State of the Art 1998. In: Fill & Mühlhäusler
(eds.), p. 43-53.
Fill,
Alwin, Hermine Penz & Wilhelm Trampe (eds.) 2002. Colourful green ideas.
Bern: Peter Lang.
Finke,
Peter. 1996. Sprache als missing link zwischen natürlichen und kulturellen
Ökosystemen. Überlegungen zur
Weiterentwicklung der Sprachökologie. In: Fill (ed.), p. 27-48.
_______.
2000. Zukunftsfähigkeit, heilige Kühe und Grammatik:
Metalinguistische Überlegungen am Ende
des Baconschen Zeitalters”. In: Kettemann & Penz (eds.), p. 63-83.
_______.
2002. Die Nachhaltigkeit der Sprache: Fünf ineinander
verschachtelte Puppen der linguistischen Ökonomie”.
In: Fill, Penz & Trampe (eds.), p. 29-58.
_______.
2008. The memory of language: New research in the beginnings of cultural
evolution. In: Döring, Martin,
Hermine Penz & Wilhelm Trampe (eds.). p. 73-88.
Garmer,
Mark. 2004. Language: An ecological view. Oxford/Berlim: Peter Lang.
Guattari, Félix. 1989. Les
trois ecologies. Paris: Galilée.
Halliday,
Michael A K., 2001. New ways of meaning – the challenge to applied linguistics.
In: Fill & Mühlhäusler (eds.),
p. 175-202.
Haugen,
Einar. 1972. The ecology of language. Stanford: Stanford University
Press: 325-339. Also in: Fill &
Mühlhäusler (eds. 2001: 57-66).
Kettemann,
Bernhard & Hermine Penz (eds.) 2000. ECOnstructing language, nature and
society. Tübingen:
Stauffenburg.
Kramsch,
Claire (ed.). 2002. Language acquisition and language socialization:
Ecological perspectives. London:
Continuum.
Leather,
Jonathan & Jet van Dam (eds.). 2003. Ecology of language acquisition.
Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Maas, Utz. 1977. Sprachliches Handeln I: Auffordern, fragen,
behaupten. In: Baumgärtner, Klaus & Hugo Steger (orgs.): Funk-Kolleg
Sprache II, p. 144-157.
Maffi,
Luisa. (ed.) 2001. On biocultural
diversity: Linking laguage, knowledge, and the environment. Washington: Smithsonian Institution
Press.
Makkai,
Adam. 1993. Ecolinguistics: ¿Toward a new **paradigm** for the
science of language? London:
Pinter Publishers.
Mufwene,
Salikoko. 2001. The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Mühlhäusler,
Peter. 2003. Language and environment – environment of language: A course in
ecolinguistics. London:
Battlebridge.
Strohner,
Hans. 1996. Die neue Systemlinguistik: Zu einer ökosystemischen
Sprachwissenschaft. In: Fill (ed). p.
49-58.
Trampe,
Wilhelm. 1990. Ökologische Linguistik: Grundlagen einer ökologischen
Wissenschafts- und Sprachtheorie.
Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.
_______. 1996.
Ökosysteme und Sprache-Welt-Syteme. In: Fill (ed.), p. 59-75.
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário